Random Xpat Rantings

Contemplative dominance for the modern man

Framing the issue: subtle tricks to get your way

Posted by xsplat on October 3, 2007

gerbil_bike.JPGIn life I want to be happy, enjoy peace, excitement, love, passion and adventure. So how can I manipulate my own mind and those around me to comply with these wishes?

I’ve learned that NLP, or careful hypnotic suggestions, are an essential component of directing attention. When I talk to my lover I often say things like “we are happy people!”. When we wake, I make a ritual happy noise, “Pagi!” (Indonesian for Good Morning). I associate our good food with our happiness and good life. Etc. If I’m cranky and angry about something, like my lover being incredibly lazy and not getting off her ass to do needed work, I try to not let the negative feedback I give her bleed all over the place and poison our fun. And I try to trick myself, or focus and modulate my own awareness in similar ways.

So this study about how framing the question affects people’s political views is powerful. The study showed that if you prime people to expect that their choice will affirm their identity and affirm their social connections, they will be more likely to agree with whatever it is you present. The facts at issue make little difference and may even work against you; (conservative) people make choices depending on how affirmed they feel.

The full article:

Nuclear Power Makes Individualists See Green

Posted by Dan Kahan on October 2nd, 2007

A while back I posted an entry about the “cultural cognition of nanotechnology risks.” The entry described a study that members of the Cultural Cognition Project at Yale Law School had done that showed that exposure to just a small bit of information about nanotechnology — a subject the vast majority of Americans have heard nothing or little about — can instantly polarize people along cultural lines. I promised that I would post another entry describing techniques for ameliorating this type of cultural polarization on risk issues.

Well, I waited about 6 months or so, not just to let suspense build but also to gather some data so that it wouldn’t seem I was just engaged in wild-eyed conjecture (although truth be told, I’m pretty partial to that mode of exposition). Now I’m going to describe a framing technique for reducing cultural polarization that involves identity affirmation (a communication strategy based on the work of social psychologist and Cultural Cognition Project researcher Geoff Cohen). And I’m going to illustrate how it works with a study that relates to global warming.

Let me start with just a little bit of background. The “cultural cognition of risk” refers to the psychological disposition of people to form beliefs about risk that cohere with their values. People who hold relatively egalitarian and communitarian values, for example, worry about environmental risks (nuclear power accidents, global warming, air pollution, etc.), the abatement of which would justify regulating commercial activities that generate inequality and legitimize the unconstrained pursuit of individual self-interest. Persons who subscribe to relatively individualistic values, in contrast, reject claims of environmental risk precisely because they cherish markets and private orderings. They worry instead that excessive gun control will render individuals unable to defend themselves — a belief congenial to the association of guns with individualist virtues such as self-reliance, courage, and martial prowess. Persons who hold traditional or hierarchical values fret about the societal risks of drug use and promiscuous sex, and the personal risks associated with obtaining an abortion or smoking marijuana — forms of behavior that denigrate traditional social norms and roles.

One of the basic mechanisms behind the cultural cognition of risk is identity-protective cognition. As a way of avoiding dissonance and estrangement from valued groups, individuals subconsciously resist factual information that threatens their defining values.

This process, though, can presumably be reversed or at least mitigated. If one can frame risk information in way that affirms rather than threatens persons’ defining commitments, then those persons should be able to process information more open-mindedly.

We (Don Braman, Situationist Contributor Paul Slovic, Geoff Cohen, John Gastil and I) decided to test this hypothesis in an experiment focusing on global warming. People with individualistic values tend to be skeptical of environmental risks like global warming because accepting such risks seems to imply that government should regulate commerce, an activity individualists like. But it turns out that one possible solution to global warming is to rely more on nuclear power, which doesn’t emit the greenhouse gas emissions associated with fossil fuel energy sources. Cultural individualists like nuclear power, because it is a symbol of individual initiative, industry, and mastery over nature. Advised that nuclear power is one solution to the risks of global warming, then, individualists should be more receptive to information suggesting that global warming in fact presents genuine and serious risks.

nuke_vs_antipollution.pngIn our experiment, we divided subjects (whose cultural values we measured ahead of time with appropriate scales) into two groups. Both received a news report describing a scientific study on global warming. The study was described as finding conclusive evidence that the temperature of the earth is increasing, that humans are the source of this condition, and that this change in the earth’s climate could have disastrous environmental economic consequences. In one version of the news story, however, the scientific study was described as calling for “increased antipollution regulation,” whereas in another it was described as calling for “revitalization of the nation’s nuclear power industry.”

The results of the experiment showed that subjects receiving the “nuclear power” version of the news story were less culturally polarized than ones receiving the “anti-pollution” version. That is, individualists who received the “nuclear power” version were less inclined to dismiss the facts related by the described report — that the earth’s temperature is increasing, that humans are the cause, and that the consequences would be dire if global warming were not reversed — than were individualists who got the “antipollution” version, even though the factual information, and its source, were the same in both articles. Indeed, individualists who received the “antipollution” version of the news report were even more skeptical about these facts than were individualists in a control group that received no newspaper story — and thus no information relating to the scientific study that made these findings.individualists_polarization.png

In sum, anti-pollution measures make individualists see red. Nuclear power makes them see green!

This is just one of a variety of experimental findings that appear in a report, “The Second National Risk and Culture Study: Making Sense of — and Progress in — the American Culture War of Fact,” released last week by the Cultural Cognition Project. The study discusses a variety of issues in addition to global warming, including domestic terrorism, school shootings, and vaccination of school-age girls for HPV. It also identifies other mechanisms for counteracting the divisive effects of cultural cognition on these and other issues. I’ll discuss some of these findings too in future posts – ones that will appear, I promise, in less than six months’ time.

But before I conclude this post, I do want to make one thing clear. The point of “identity affirmation” and like techniques for counteracting cultural cognition is not to induce people to believe any particular set of facts about climate change, gun control, anti-terrorism policies, or the like. Rather it’s to neutralize the tendency of people to polarize along cultural lines as they consider and discuss information about such matters. If society availed itself of risk-communication and regulatory strategies founded on “identity affirmation” and similar mechanisms, disagreements about facts would no doubt persist, but they would no longer take the form of battles between rival cultural factions. And we think that’s a good thing.

-o-o-o-o-o-o-o-o-o-o-

Update:

I wonder if framing the issue to bias identity affirmation has as powerful effect on people who hold relatively egalitarian and communitarian values.

There is evidence that conservatism in general has some genetic components. And it may be that people who are more open to change think differently than those who tend to be more fearful of it, and so they may be less influenced by threat to their identity and their in-group.

Bob Altmeyer has interesting things to say about conservative thinking here . I question differences in the two groups (idiot conservatives and smart people) in my blog post Timid choices lead to Muslims, Republicans, and boring people.

I’d be curious to hear about studies comparing liberals and conservatives regarding how influenced they are by framing.

Update: Dan Kahan was kind enough to give this info:

“I noticed your curiosity about whether liberals & conservatives respond to “affirmation framings.” Geoff Cohen’s work is on-point for this; many of his studies seek to use “affirmation” to counteract identity-protective cogntion where relevant identity is “liberal” or “conservative.” Articles include:
Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party over Policy: The Dominating Impact of Group Influence on Political Beliefs. J. Personality & Soc. Psych., 85(5), 808-822
Cohen, G. L., Aronson, J., & Steele, C. M. (2000). When Beliefs Yield to Evidence: Reducing Biased Evaluation by Affirming the Self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(9), 1151-1164.
Cohen, G. L., Sherman, D. K., Bastardi, A., Hsu, L., & McGoey, M. (in press). Bridging the Partisan Divide: Self-Affirmation Reduces Ideological Closed-Mindedness and Inflexibility in Negotiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

Also see his description of this line of work at See http://psych.colorado.edu/%7Egcohen/bias-jdm.html .
I think his work– and the work he’s done w/ us using cultural worldviews (which we have reason to believe supply much more robust sources of individual identity than “liberal” and “conservative,” particularly for people of relatively modest political “sophistication” or knowledge) — cut against the idea that one or another type (liberal conservative, hierarch, individualist, egalitarian, communitarian, etc.) is more or less open to influence of framing & related techniques on processing of information. “

Update: I can’t find Geoff Cohen’s papers online.  Next week I’ll email him with a request (I’ll be away).  In the meantime, my bias pulls me to what I identify with; that free thinkers and squares are different.

About these ads

One Response to “Framing the issue: subtle tricks to get your way”

  1. Joey Giraud said

    When talking gun control with my right-wing cousins, talked a bit about hunting first, then about calibres and rifling. My older cuz admits that handguns only good for killing people. Word.

Leave a Reply

Fill in your details below or click an icon to log in:

WordPress.com Logo

You are commenting using your WordPress.com account. Log Out / Change )

Twitter picture

You are commenting using your Twitter account. Log Out / Change )

Facebook photo

You are commenting using your Facebook account. Log Out / Change )

Google+ photo

You are commenting using your Google+ account. Log Out / Change )

Connecting to %s

 
Follow

Get every new post delivered to your Inbox.

Join 110 other followers

%d bloggers like this: